2 Morpho-Phonology | 7a Using Affixes | 12 The Number System | |
3 Basic Morphology | 7b Using Affixes (continued) | The Lexicon | |
4 Case Morphology | 8 Adjuncts | Revised Ithkuil: Ilaksh |
Modality corresponds roughly to the function of both modal verbs in Western languages (e.g., can, may, must, should, etc.) as well as those verbs which modify a following verb such as to want to, to choose to, to need to, to offer to, to demand that, etc. However, in Ithkuil, the effect of such modifications on a verb causes a fundamental change in the cognitive interpretation of the verb, usually resulting in a modification of both the Essence (see Sec. 3.8) and the Perspective (see Sec. 3.3) of the verb, as well as invoking the use of the ACTIVATIVE case to mark the subject noun (see Sec. 4.3.9). The nature of these modifications is explained as follows:
As we saw in Sections 3.8 and 4.3.9, it is possible in human language to speak about events that are either unreal, as-yet-unrealized, or alternative versions of reality. Specifically, nouns and verbs can make reference to hypothetical representations of real-world counterparts from within an alternative mental space created psychologically (and implied linguistically). This alternative mental space is essentially the psychological realm of potential and imagination. It is seen, for example, in the following sentences.
1) You must come home at once.
2) That girl can sing better than anybody.
3) Our troops should attack at dawn.
4) Mother needs you to come with her.
5) The teacher requests that I dance for you.
6) The man believes clowns are dangerous.
Each of the above sentences describe potential or unreal events, not actual real-world happenings that are occurring or have occurred. In Sentence (1) no one has yet come home, in Sentence (2) the girl may choose never to sing again, Sentence (3) does not tell us whether any attack will actually occur, Sentence (4) does not indicate whether you will come or not, Sentence (5) does not indicate whether I will dance, nor does Sentence (6) establish whether or not clowns are, in fact, dangerous.
Because the clauses following the verbs must, can, should, need, request, believe, in the above sentences all refer to unrealized, imagined, or hypothetical events, the nouns and verbs within those clauses would be marked in the ABSTRACT perspective (see Sec. 3.4) and the REPRESENTATIVE essence (see Sec. 3.8). The subject nouns which invoke the event (the nouns you, girl, troops, mother, teacher, and man in the six sentences above) would be marked in the ACTIVATIVE case (see Sec. 4.3.9). It should be noted that not all Ithkuil modalities necessarily invoke hypothetical or unrealized events. For example, in the sentence She chose to move to Australia, the verb chose signals that the following clause is spoken of abstractly (i.e., it is the act of choice that is being talked about, not the move itself), but nevertheless refers to an actual event (i.e., she did, in fact, move to Australia). Thus, the move to Australia clause would be marked in the ABSTRACT perspective but would not be marked in the REPRESENTATIVE essence. Thus, the requirement that an Ithkuil modality construction invoke modifications in the perspective, essence and case of the associated nouns and verbs is entirely dependent on the semantics and cognitive intent of the utterance.
There are 30 modalities in Ithkuil. Modality is marked by a word-final vocalic suffix to a conflational or valence adjunct (see Secs. 5.2 and 5.4). In the absence of a conflational or valence adjunct, this vocalic suffix can stand alone as an autonomous word, functioning as a modality adjunct. The thirty modalities and their respective suffixes are described below.
5.5.1 | DES |
The Desiderative |
The DESIDERATIVE more or less corresponds to English constructions expressing desire, e.g., to want to, to desire that, etc. as in The teacher wants the students to study hard. It is marked by adding the suffix -a to a conflational or valence adjunct.
5.5.2 | ASP |
The Aspirative |
The ASPIRATIVE corresponds to English constructions expressing wishing or hoping, e.g., to wish that, to hope that, etc. It is marked by adding the suffix -ü to a conflational or valence adjunct.
5.5.3 | EPC |
The Expectative |
The EXPECTATIVE corresponds to English constructions expressing expectation, as in He expected her to be beautiful, or I imagine hes reached his destination. It is marked by adding the suffix -ï to a conflational or valence adjunct.
5.5.4 | CRD |
The Credential |
The CREDENTIAL corresponds to English constructions expressing belief, as in I think she has two sons, or We believe the earth to be round. Note that it does not correspond to English expressions of opinion, i.e., it would not be used in translating sentences such as I think shes pretty. It is marked by adding the suffix -u to a conflational or valence adjunct.
5.5.5 | REQ |
The Requisitive |
The REQUISITIVE corresponds to English
constructions expressing requests, as in I request his presence at the banquet,
or Id like you to visit your father. It is marked by adding
the suffix -â to a conflational or valence adjunct.
5.5.6 | EXH |
The Exhortative |
The EXHORTATIVE corresponds to English expressions of exhortation or demand, as in I demand you return my book or Let the gates be opened! It is marked by adding the suffix -û to a conflational or valence adjunct.
5.5.7 | OPR |
The Opportunitive |
The OPPORTUNITIVE corresponds to the English modal verb can/could/be able where it means have the opportunity to, as in Can we pass by our old house when we visit town? or Because of the delay, she was able to go to the museum after all. It is marked by adding the suffix -ai to a conflational or valence adjunct.
5.5.8 | CPC |
The Capacitative |
The CAPACITATIVE corresponds to the English modal verb can/could/be able where it means have the ability or capacity to, as in Can she sing opera? or He could run like the wind. Note that it would also be used when translating English generic statements implying ability or capacity as in He speaks French like a native [i.e., the sentence does not imply he is speaking French at the moment, but rather his general ability to do so]. It is marked by adding the suffix -ei to a conflational or valence adjunct.
5.5.9 | PRM |
The Permissive |
The PERMISSIVE corresponds to the English modal verbs can/could/be able or may/might where they mean be permitted to, as in Very well, you can have ice cream for dessert; or Could I talk to you? It is marked by adding the suffix -oi to a conflational or valence adjunct.
5.5.10 | PTN |
The Potential |
The POTENTIAL corresponds to the English modal verb can/could/be able where it means have the potential to or the possibility of, as in Remember it can flood suddenly in this area, or That man could fly into rages for no reason. It is also used when translating generic statements implying potential or possibility, as in It rains unpredictably in the Northwest. It is marked by adding the suffix -ëi to a conflational or valence adjunct.
5.5.11 | CLS |
The Compulsory |
The COMPULSORY corresponds to the English modal verbs must or have to/had to in their meaning of compulsory obligation, as in You must get up now, or I had to attend the ceremony. It is marked by adding the suffix -ui to a conflational or valence adjunct.
5.5.12 | OBG |
The Obligative |
The OBLIGATIVE corresponds to the English modal verbs should, ought to, or other phrases expressing optional but preferential obligation, as in You ought to see a dentist, I should tell her how I feel, or It would be best if the children stayed away from clowns. It is marked by adding the suffix -iu to a conflational or valence adjunct.
5.5.13 | IMS |
The Impositive |
The IMPOSITIVE corresponds to English expressions such as be supposed to, be expected to, or be to which impose an expectation upon a party, as in Youre supposed to smile when introduced, or He is to give a speech at the banquet. It is marked by adding the suffix -au to a conflational or valence adjunct.
5.5.14 | ADV |
The Advocative |
The ADVOCATIVE corresponds to English expressions such as suggest that or propose that which advocate a position, as in I suggest you study harder or They proposed that the clowns take their circus tent elsewhere. It is marked by adding the suffix -ia to a conflational or valence adjunct.
5.5.15 | ITV |
The Intentive |
The INTENTIVE corresponds to English expressions such as intend to, plan on or shall which convey an intention, as in The girls plan on travelling to Europe, or I shall see to it. It is marked by adding the suffix -ou to a conflational or valence adjunct.
5.5.16 | ANT |
The Anticipative |
The ANTICIPATIVE corresponds to English expressions such as to look forward to or to eagerly await which convey positive anticipation, as in We look forward to the clowns coming to town. It is marked by addition of the suffix -eu to a conflational or valence adjunct.
5.5.17 | DSP |
The Dispositive |
The DISPOSITIVE corresponds to the English expression to be willing to, conveying willingness as in He is willing to give his life to defeat the clowns. It is marked by addition of the suffix -äi to a conflational or valence adjunct.
5.5.18 | PRE |
The Preparative |
The PREPARATIVE corresponds to the English expressions be ready to or be prepared to, indicating readiness, as in Shes ready to host the party, or They are prepared to endure harsh weather. It is marked by addition of the suffix -öi to a conflational or valence adjunct.
5.5.19 | NEC |
The Necessitative |
The NECESSITATIVE corresponds to the English expressions need to or be necessary to, indicating necessity, as in You need to do something about those clowns in the yard, or It was necessary to inform her about the atrocities. It is marked by addition of the suffix -ëu to a conflational or valence adjunct.
5.5.20 | DEC |
The Decisive |
The DECISIVE corresponds to English expressions such as decide to or choose to, indicating choice, as in Peter decided to cook breakfast or Colleen chose to visit the clowns. It is marked by addition of the suffix -aï to a conflational or valence adjunct.
5.5.21 | PTV |
The Proclivitive |
The PROCLIVITIVE corresponds to English expressions such as tend to, be apt to, or be prone to, indicating tendency, as in The wolverine tended to eat platypus eggs, or Boris is apt to make a fool of himself when meeting women. It is marked by addition of the suffix -eï to a conflational or valence adjunct.
5.5.22 | VOL |
The Voluntative |
The VOLUNTATIVE corresponds to English expressions such as offer to or volunteer to, indicating an act of offering as in The foreman offered to put poison in my beer, or Mrs. Blathermot volunteered to bake artichoke pie for the Open House event. It is marked by addition of the suffix -iï to a conflational or valence adjunct.
5.5.23 | ACC |
The Accordative |
The ACCORDATIVE corresponds to the English expression agree to, as in Constance agreed to perform her go-go number at the talent show. It is marked by addition of the suffix -oï to a conflational or valence adjunct.
5.5.24 | INC |
The Inclinative |
The INCLINATIVE corresponds to English expressions such as to feel like or be up for, indicating an impulsive desire, as in Hes up for going to the shindig, or Molly felt like eating the entire chocolate cake. It is marked by addition of the suffix -öu to a conflational or valence adjunct.
5.5.25 | CML |
The Compulsive |
The COMPULSIVE corresponds to English expressions such as feel driven to or feel a need to, indicating compulsion, as in Jack feels driven to hunt wolverines, or My hairdresser feels a need to date bikers. It is marked by addition of the suffix -uï to a conflational or valence adjunct.
5.5.26 | DVR |
The Divertive |
The DIVERTIVE corresponds to English expressions such as like to, or enjoy, conveying simple likes and pasttimes as in Boys like to dream about cars, or My salamander enjoyed biting people on their rump. It is marked by addition of the suffix -äu to a conflational or valence adjunct.
5.5.27 | DVT |
The Devotive |
The DEVOTIVE corresponds to English expressions such as be devoted to or be committed to, indicating devotion as in Charlotte is committed to being the best seamstress in town, or They were devoted to rooting for the losing team. It is marked by addition of the suffix -ëï to a conflational or valence adjunct.
5.5.28 | PFT |
The Preferential |
The PREFERENTIAL corresponds to English expressions such as prefer, or would rather that, indicating preference as in Hed rather work by himself, or Muldane prefers that his cats eat live food. It is marked by addition of the suffix -ua to a conflational or valence adjunct.
5.5.29 | IPS |
The Impressional |
The IMPRESSIONAL corresponds to English expressions such as have an impression that, have a hunch that, or feel that, indicating a subjective belief or impression as in Ive a hunch that the porter is an alcoholic, or Betty feels her husband flirts too much with the secretarial pool. It is marked by addition of the suffix -üa to a conflational or valence adjunct.
5.5.30 | PMS |
The Promissory |
The PROMISSORY corresponds to English expressions such as promise, or swear that, indicating a self-imposed obligation as in She promised that her son would visit my daughter, or Hargreaves swears that the fish from that lake are sentient. It is marked by addition of the suffix -iù to a conflational or valence adjunct.
Level corresponds roughly to what is known as degree of comparison in other languages. Many languages morphologically indicate degrees of comparison as exemplified by the English suffixes -er and -est seen in great-greater-greatest. Ithkuil incorporates degrees of comparison morphologically into its valence scheme by means of tone of the conflational adjunct (or valence or modality adjunct). Specifically, the four tones are used to show four different comparisons, each of which is termed a level of the verb. These levels are the INDETERMINATE, EQUATIVE, SURPASSIVE and DEFICIENT, and are explained below.
5.6.1 | IDT |
The Indeterminate |
Shown by low or falling tone of the conflational, valence or modality adjunct. The INDETERMINATE is the default level and indicates either 1) that no comparison is being made between the first party and the second party, 2) that the degree of comparison between the first party and the second party is unknown or irrelevant, or 3) that comparison is inapplicable because the verb is monoactive (i.e., there is only one party participating).
5.6.2 | EQU |
The Equative |
Shown by high tone of the conflational, valence or modality adjunct. The EQUATIVE level indicates that the first party performs its half of the action equally well in comparison to the second party. It corresponds to the English construction as [well] as as in She sings as well as I do or He reads as fast as you do.
5.6.3 | SUR |
The Surpassive |
Shown by rising tone of the conflational, valence or modality adjunct. The SURPASSIVE level indicates that the first party manifests a state or performs an action to a greater degree or extent than the second party. It functions equivalently to English -er as in I ran farther than he. It also corresponds to the English verbal prefix out- as in I out-sang him (i.e., I sang longer or better than he), although the SURPASSIVE is more flexible than the English construction, as it can be used with any of the thirteen valences of the verb. For example, when used with the NONRELATIONAL valence for a verb such as laugh, it would translate something like I laughed more/harder than anyone did anything else. It can even be used with the MONOACTIVE valence, indicating that the party performed the action superlatively and no one else did. For example, the sentence I laughed in the MONOACTIVE valence and SURPASSIVE level would translate as something like I was the only one laughing and my laughter was superlative.
The SURPASSIVE can also be used to translate the English -est superlative construction, as in She is the nicest (of all) by simply naming a contextually universal second participant to the verb, i.e., She out-nices everyone else.
5.6.4 | DFT |
The Deficient |
Shown by broken tone of the conflational, valence or modality adjunct. The DEFICIENT level corresponds roughly to the negative comparative less in English as in This test is less difficult, as well as the verbal prefix under- as in He under-performed tonight. Like the other levels, it can be used with any valence of the Ithkuil verb to indicate that the performance or state of the first participant to a co-active verb is worse or less than that of the second participant. It would thus render sentences corresponding to She under-danced tonight (i.e., she didnt dance as well as she might have) or The boy under-weighs the girl (i.e., he weighs less than her).
Virtually all languages allow for sentences to be hierarchically embedded within other sentences, a process termed subordination. In Western languages, the embedded sentence becomes either a subordinate clause or a relative clause, explicitly introduced by a conjunctions such as that, which, who, where,although,if,while,whereas, or a preposition followed by a conjunction, such as through which,by whom,etc. In English, such clauses can also occur as an infinitive or gerundial verb construction. Both relative and subordinate clauses are illustrated in the following sentences:
The dog that ate my hat belongs to them.
I want him to stop shouting.
The committee voted to fire the superintendant.
We demand (that) you give us equal pay.
Although hes a college graduate, he acts like a child.
This is the slot through which the letter is passed.
In case youre unaware, Ill be leaving next month.
The boy walking toward us is my nephew.
The Ithkuil equivalent to relative or subordinate clauses is known as a case-frame, or simply, frame. Conceptually, the sentence to be embedded is simply treated as a noun participant to the main verb of a sentence and is therefore marked for case like any other noun. For example, take the following two sentences:
She and I were working together.
The two nations were at war.
Suppose we want to use the second sentence to provide a temporal context for the first sentence. In English we could do this by subordinating the second sentence to the first using the conjunction while, as in She and I were working together while the two nations were at war. Alternately, we could create a relative clause by inserting a connecting prepositional phrase, as in She and I were working together during the time (that) the two nations were at war.
In Ithkuil, temporal context for a sentence may be provided by a noun in any of the temporal cases such as the CONCURSIVE (see Sec. 4.9.1). A word such as summer or famine would be placed in the CONCURSIVE case to create a sentence corresponding to:
She and I were working together during the summer.
She and I were working together at the time of the famine.
Just as the single words summer and famine are placed in the CONCURSIVE case, so an entire sentence such as The two nations were at war can be placed in the CONCURSIVE case to provide the temporal context for the main sentence. In other words, Ithkuil treats the entire subordinate sentence as a noun phrase to be declined into any required case. That is the purpose of a frame, to place sentences into noun cases. By doing so, Ithkuil accomplishes the same task for which Western languages use relative and subordinate conjunctions. In theory, any sentence can be placed into any of the 81 cases and inserted into another sentence wherever a simple noun might be placed in the sentence using that same case.
To construct a case-frame, the second-order sentence (i.e., the sentence to be subordinated) is placed in the main sentence at the point where a noun declined for the required case would appear. Unlike the usual verb-final word order of main sentences in Ithkuil, a case-frame usually has its verb appear as the first element of the case frame. This is to easily recognize the beginning of the case frame. The actual case of the sentence is indicated in the verbal formative in the same way as for nominal formatives, i.e., by mutation of the stem vowel and mutation of the C2 radical consonant. Table 17 below offers a review of these mutations for each of the 81 cases. If inserted into the middle of the main sentence, the final word of the case-frame should be a noun (or a personal reference adjunct see Sec. 8.1) and carries a special affix which signifies the end of the frame if this will help to avoid confusion as to which words in the sentence belong inside the frame (i.e., with the secondary sentence), and which belong to the main sentence.
Table 17: Case-Frame Markers
(Mutation by Vowel Series and C2 Grade)
|
|
In general, the perspective of the verb in the secondary sentence operates independently from that of the main verb, however, it is also common for the perspective of the verb in the secondary sentence to be placed in the ABSTRACT, which has the effect of deferring all Perspective information about the verb to the main verb, similarly to the way English subordinate clauses using gerunds and infinitives defer all tense information to the main verb of the sentence.
Here is an example Ithkuil sentence containing a case frame:
There is no direct equivalent in Ithkuil to the relative clauses of Western languages. Ithkuil treats such clauses the same as subordinate clauses using case-frames as described above. However, the manner in which this is done, while ultimately logical, is somewhat complex and confusing from a Western perspective. Therefore, to analyze how Ithkuil reinterprets Western relative clauses into subordinate case-frames will first require us to review the nature of relative clauses in Western languages such as English.
A relative clause refers to an imbedded sentence which modifies or describes a head noun in the main clause. There are two types of relative clauses, restricted (or dependent) and unrestricted (or independent). The two types are illustrated in the following English sentences.
RESTRICTED CLAUSE
(1) Lions that like chasing their tails can be seen at
any circus.
(2) That book (that) I just finished reading was written
by a priest.
UNRESTRICTED CLAUSE
(3) Lions, which like chasing their tails, can be seen
at any circus.
(4) That book, which I just finished reading, was written
by a priest.
In the first sentence, the clause that like chasing their tails refers to a specific type of lion found at a circus (i.e, not all lions chase their tails). Similarly, the clause (that) I just finished reading in the second sentence is restricted in that it is considered by the speaker as being necessary in order to identify which book is being talked about, i.e., without the clause, the listener would not know which book the speaker was referring to.
Note the difference in meaning, however, when comparing the first two sentences to the third and fourth sentences. In the third sentence, the speaker implies that all lions chase their tails regardless of whether they are in the circus. In the fourth sentence, the identity of the book is already known to the listener, and the speaker is merely providing two additional facts about it: the fact that he just finished reading it and the fact about its author. Notice that in English, an unrestricted relative clause is set off in writing by commas and cannot begin with that (rather which or who must be used); also, such clauses are normally spoken in a lowered intonation with juncture (i.e., brief pauses) immediately before and after the clause.
5.7.2.1 Restricted Clauses. Ithkuil treats the above notions
about relative clauses in a different way. We will first analyze how Ithkuil
creates equivalents to restricted relative clauses. This can best be approached
by analyzing the underlying sentences which give rise to the main and relative
clauses. Analyzing Sentence No. 2 above, it can be broken up into two discrete
sentences:
That book was written by a priest. (= A priest wrote that
book.)
I just finished reading that book.
In Ithkuil, the sentence which will be functioning as the main sentence acts as a template in which the secondary sentence is placed. The particular place in the template to be filled is dependent on what semantic role, i.e., case (see Chapter 4) the secondary sentence is to fill. Note that the common point of reference of the two sentences is that book. In the main sentence, that book functions in the semantic role of CONTENT (See Sec. 4.1.2), superficially equivalent to the direct object of the ABSOLUTIVE subject priest, therefore, the main sentence becomes the template A priest wrote X where X is in the OBLIQUE case (See Section 4.3.1). Meanwhile, in the secondary sentence, the noun which is the common point of reference (what in Western grammar would be called the head of the relative clause) is marked with an affix indicating such. So we now have the two sentences as:
A priest wrote [ ]. I just finish reading that book-H.
The -H in the second sentence above is meant to represent an affix marking the head or common reference point between the two sentences. At this point, Ithkuil inserts the second sentence as a case-frame into the empty slot based on the semantic role it will be playing, in this instance the role of CONTENT marked by the OBLIQUE case (see Sec. 4.3.1).
A priest wrote [OBL]. I just finish reading that book-H.
As described in Sec. 5.7.1, the verb of the secondary sentence is moved to the beginning of the case-frame and takes the relevant case marker (OBLIQUE).
A priest wrote I just finished reading-OBL that book-H.
Reverse translating this sentence back to English, the closest literal translation would be the rather awkward construction: A priest wrote what I just finished reading, that book. However, this is how Ithkuil translates the English sentence A priest wrote that book that I just finished reading.
Two observations can be noted from the Ithkuil sentence. First of all, unlike Western languages, the main clause contains no head. Instead, the head is marked from within the imbedded clause. Secondly, there is no difference between this process and the rendering of other types of subordinate clauses using case-frames, as the main sentence was rearranged (or reinterpreted) to provide a slot for the semantic role of the imbedded sentence, the exact same way that subordinate clauses are constructed in Ithkuil. Therefore, as was previously stated, Ithkuil makes no distinction between subordinate and relative clauses.
Similarly, the other example sentence from above, Lions that like chasing their tails can be seen at any circus would become in Ithkuil: At any circus one can see certain lions-H like to chase-OBL their tails. A literal translation into English would be: At any circus one can see (that) certain lions like chasing their tails.
5.7.2.2 Unrestricted Clauses. As for independent or unrestricted
clauses, as shown in example sentences (3) and (4) earlier, Ithkuil treats these
differently still. In Western languages, an unrestricted clause does not help
to identify a noun or provide a context for it, but simply adds additional information
about an already identified noun. Thus, unrestricted relative clauses serve
a wholly different cognitive-semantic purpose than restricted clauses, a fact
hidden by their nearly identical surface structures. Ithkuil acknowledges this
profound difference at the overt sentence level by not subordinating any clause
at all. Rather, the two sentences are given co-equal status as main clauses
and simply joined by a coordinating affix. Thus sentences (3) and (4) from earlier
become:
One can see lions at any circus and they like chasing their tails.
A priest wrote that book and I just finished reading it.
5.7.2.3 Use of the INHERENT Case In Lieu of Simple
Relative Clauses. In Section
4.5.3 we analyzed how MONADIC or UNBOUNDED
nouns in the INHERENT case take on a special function
of existential identification corresponding to the English phrase that/which/who
is/are
to render forms structured as the my-wife woman,
meaning the woman who is my wife. This construction allows a short-cut
way of rendering what in English would be simple relative clauses involving
descriptive identifications, eliminating the need in most cases to resort to
a case-frame. For example, the sentence The man who came to dinner is my
father is rendered as The my-father man came to dinner,
where the noun father would be in the INHERENT plus POSITIVE
focus (see Sec.
3.5) to indicate that it is the new information being conveyed by the speaker,
i.e., to distinguish it from the reversed focus of The man who is my father
came to dinner.
2 Morpho-Phonology | 7a Using Affixes | 12 The Number System | |
3 Basic Morphology | 7b Using Affixes (continued) | The Lexicon | |
4 Case Morphology | 8 Adjuncts | Revised Ithkuil: Ilaksh |
©2004-2009 by John Quijada. You may copy or excerpt any portion of the contents of this website provided you give full attribution to the author and this website.